adverbs - "Here he comes", "Here comes he" : The order of pronoun and verb in inversion
It's very common to say:
"Here he comes."
"Here comes the man."
But what about:
"Here comes he."
"Here the man comes."
Is there a rule about the order of noun and verb in inversion?
It will be too much if all the inversions are considered, so let us talk about adverb inversions only, such as:
"Here comes the rainbow."
"Off we go."
"In the room is the man lying."
Answer
Information packaging constraints
Here comes he.
This sentence would usually be considered malformed, whereas Here comes Bob would be considered perfectly correct. Both are cases of what is known as Subject-dependent inversion.
Subject-dependent inversion has to do with the linguistic concept of information packaging. In other words it is about how we arrange the order of words and phrases in a sentence.
The vast majority of non-canonical phrase orderings in English are due to the status of particular words and concepts in terms of the current discourse, in particular, whether items are considered as old or new in the conversation. Most importantly they make use of the fact that we prefer to put things that have already been discussed in the recent discourse at the beginning of the sentence. New concepts and information tend to come at the end. It is the end of the sentence which carries the most emphasis, not the beginning.
A general constraint on the felicity of most types of phrase reordering, therefore, is that phrases that are preposed should if possible be discourse old (already have been talked about) or be in the speaker's or listener's immediate vicinity. The general motivation for doing this is so that some other phrase will then appear towards or at the end of the sentence where it will receive the most emphasis, and be easier for the listener to process. For this reason there is an even stronger constraint on elements which are propelled from their canonical position towards the end of the sentence: these items should usually be discourse new. They should not already have been being talked about. There are, of course, some exceptions to this 'rule'. For example, items that the speaker wants to give contrastive stress to may be moved to sentence final position, where they will receive the most emphasis.
Locative and temporal adjuncts (read adverbials) buck this general trend, in that whilst putting new information at the beginning of a sentence seems to put a lot of strain on the listener, the preposing of locative complements seems to make it easier to process sentences where nearly all the information is new.
These types of considerations are usually what motivates the use of (long) passives, existential constructions, so-called it clefts and, of course, subject-dependent inversion. Compare the following pairs, where the first has a canonical phrase ordering and the second uses some alternative form of information packaging:
a. I've been studying the Mona Lisa. Leonardo Da Vinci painted the Mona Lisa.
b. I've been studying the Mona Lisa. The Mona Lisa was painted by Leonardo Da Vinci.
The (b) example here is better. The first is rather stilted. The reason the passive sentence works well is that the Mona Lisa links back to the first sentence, whilst Da Vinci, which is new to the discourse, takes the prominent position at the end.
a. A cafe is on the corner.
b. There's a cafe on the corner.
c. On the corner is a cafe.
Example (a) feels very awkward here. The reason is that a cafe is new information. We know this because of the use of the indefinite article, "a". It's awkward and difficult to process in this position. Both (b) and (c) examples are fine. The existential, there is, construction gives the reader a split second to anticipate the introduction of a new entity into the discourse. The (c) example uses subject dependent inversion. This places the locative adjunct, which is relatively easy to process, at the beginning of the sentence. The phrase a cafe, in contrast, gets transported to the end of the sentence where it receives the most emphasis (compare the prominence of cafe in this example, with its relatively weaker status in the existential).
Here comes he.
The reason that this sentence will strike most readers as odd is that he occurs in a very prominent position at the end of the sentence. This position would normally be reserved for new entities, especially if we have specifically changed the normal canonical word order of the sentence. The fact that we are using a pronoun here means that we must already have been talking about this man in the previous discourse. Having a discourse old word shunted into this position goes against our natural language expectations and seems to jar. However, we might possibly be able to manufacture a scenario where this might be acceptable. For example imagine the following exchange:
- The only person who could save us now is Bob.
- Oh, look! Here comes he!
In the exchange above, we might possibly consider the sentence grammatical. The reason for this is that in some sense Bob really is arriving anew in the discourse. In the original sentence Bob was a displaced entity, far from the immediate environment. Bob's entry into the immediate vicinity if the speakers make him new and newsworthy again. In any case, whether this is the main reason for the increased acceptability of this inverted sentence, the fact is that Bob is centre stage in this sentence and can therefore be moved to this emphatic position.
Comments
Post a Comment